-
Notifications
You must be signed in to change notification settings - Fork 2
/
file.bib
3261 lines (2996 loc) · 336 KB
/
file.bib
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
61
62
63
64
65
66
67
68
69
70
71
72
73
74
75
76
77
78
79
80
81
82
83
84
85
86
87
88
89
90
91
92
93
94
95
96
97
98
99
100
101
102
103
104
105
106
107
108
109
110
111
112
113
114
115
116
117
118
119
120
121
122
123
124
125
126
127
128
129
130
131
132
133
134
135
136
137
138
139
140
141
142
143
144
145
146
147
148
149
150
151
152
153
154
155
156
157
158
159
160
161
162
163
164
165
166
167
168
169
170
171
172
173
174
175
176
177
178
179
180
181
182
183
184
185
186
187
188
189
190
191
192
193
194
195
196
197
198
199
200
201
202
203
204
205
206
207
208
209
210
211
212
213
214
215
216
217
218
219
220
221
222
223
224
225
226
227
228
229
230
231
232
233
234
235
236
237
238
239
240
241
242
243
244
245
246
247
248
249
250
251
252
253
254
255
256
257
258
259
260
261
262
263
264
265
266
267
268
269
270
271
272
273
274
275
276
277
278
279
280
281
282
283
284
285
286
287
288
289
290
291
292
293
294
295
296
297
298
299
300
301
302
303
304
305
306
307
308
309
310
311
312
313
314
315
316
317
318
319
320
321
322
323
324
325
326
327
328
329
330
331
332
333
334
335
336
337
338
339
340
341
342
343
344
345
346
347
348
349
350
351
352
353
354
355
356
357
358
359
360
361
362
363
364
365
366
367
368
369
370
371
372
373
374
375
376
377
378
379
380
381
382
383
384
385
386
387
388
389
390
391
392
393
394
395
396
397
398
399
400
401
402
403
404
405
406
407
408
409
410
411
412
413
414
415
416
417
418
419
420
421
422
423
424
425
426
427
428
429
430
431
432
433
434
435
436
437
438
439
440
441
442
443
444
445
446
447
448
449
450
451
452
453
454
455
456
457
458
459
460
461
462
463
464
465
466
467
468
469
470
471
472
473
474
475
476
477
478
479
480
481
482
483
484
485
486
487
488
489
490
491
492
493
494
495
496
497
498
499
500
501
502
503
504
505
506
507
508
509
510
511
512
513
514
515
516
517
518
519
520
521
522
523
524
525
526
527
528
529
530
531
532
533
534
535
536
537
538
539
540
541
542
543
544
545
546
547
548
549
550
551
552
553
554
555
556
557
558
559
560
561
562
563
564
565
566
567
568
569
570
571
572
573
574
575
576
577
578
579
580
581
582
583
584
585
586
587
588
589
590
591
592
593
594
595
596
597
598
599
600
601
602
603
604
605
606
607
608
609
610
611
612
613
614
615
616
617
618
619
620
621
622
623
624
625
626
627
628
629
630
631
632
633
634
635
636
637
638
639
640
641
642
643
644
645
646
647
648
649
650
651
652
653
654
655
656
657
658
659
660
661
662
663
664
665
666
667
668
669
670
671
672
673
674
675
676
677
678
679
680
681
682
683
684
685
686
687
688
689
690
691
692
693
694
695
696
697
698
699
700
701
702
703
704
705
706
707
708
709
710
711
712
713
714
715
716
717
718
719
720
721
722
723
724
725
726
727
728
729
730
731
732
733
734
735
736
737
738
739
740
741
742
743
744
745
746
747
748
749
750
751
752
753
754
755
756
757
758
759
760
761
762
763
764
765
766
767
768
769
770
771
772
773
774
775
776
777
778
779
780
781
782
783
784
785
786
787
788
789
790
791
792
793
794
795
796
797
798
799
800
801
802
803
804
805
806
807
808
809
810
811
812
813
814
815
816
817
818
819
820
821
822
823
824
825
826
827
828
829
830
831
832
833
834
835
836
837
838
839
840
841
842
843
844
845
846
847
848
849
850
851
852
853
854
855
856
857
858
859
860
861
862
863
864
865
866
867
868
869
870
871
872
873
874
875
876
877
878
879
880
881
882
883
884
885
886
887
888
889
890
891
892
893
894
895
896
897
898
899
900
901
902
903
904
905
906
907
908
909
910
911
912
913
914
915
916
917
918
919
920
921
922
923
924
925
926
927
928
929
930
931
932
933
934
935
936
937
938
939
940
941
942
943
944
945
946
947
948
949
950
951
952
953
954
955
956
957
958
959
960
961
962
963
964
965
966
967
968
969
970
971
972
973
974
975
976
977
978
979
980
981
982
983
984
985
986
987
988
989
990
991
992
993
994
995
996
997
998
999
1000
% Encoding: UTF-8
@InProceedings{Turton2017,
author = {Terece L. Turton and Colin Ware and Francesca Samsel and David H. Rogers},
title = {A Crowdsourced Approach to Colormap Assessment},
booktitle = {EuroVis Workshop on Reproducibility, Verification, and Validation in Visualization (EuroRV3)},
year = {2017},
editor = {Kai Lawonn and Noeska Smit and Douglas Cunningham},
publisher = {The Eurographics Association},
abstract = {Despite continual research and discussion on the perceptual effects of color in scientific visualization, psychophysical testing is often limited. In-person lab studies can be expensive and time-consuming while results can be difficult to extrapolate from meticulously controlled laboratory conditions to the real world of the visualization user. We draw on lessons learned from the use of crowdsourced participant pools in the behavioral sciences and information visualization to apply a crowdsourced approach to a classic psychophysical experiment assessing the ability of a colormap to impart metric information. We use an online presentation analogous to the color key task from Ware's 1988 paper, Color Sequences for Univariate Maps, testing colormaps similar to those in the original paper along with contemporary colormap standards and new alternatives in the scientific visualization domain. We explore the issue of potential contamination from color deficient participants and establish that perceptual color research can appropriately leverage a crowdsourced participant pool without significant CVD concerns. The updated version of the Ware color key task also provides a method to assess and compare colormaps.},
doi = {10.2312/eurorv3.20171106},
isbn = {978-3-03868-041-3},
}
@InProceedings{Wigdor2007,
author = {Daniel Wigdor and Chia Shen and Clifton Forlines and Ravin Balakrishnan},
title = {Perception of elementary graphical elements in tabletop and multi-surface environments},
booktitle = {Proceedings of the {SIGCHI} conference on Human factors in computing systems - {CHI} '07},
year = {2007},
publisher = {{ACM} Press},
abstract = {Information shown on a tabletop display can appear distorted when viewed by a seated user. Even worse, the impact of this distortion is different depending on the location of the information on the display. In this paper, we examine how this distortion affects the perception of the basic graphical elements of information visualization shown on displays at various angles. We first examine perception of these elements on a single display, and then compare this to perception across displays, in order to evaluate the effectiveness of various elements for use in a tabletop and multi-display environment. We found that the perception of some graphical elements is more robust to distortion than others. We then develop recommendations for building data visualizations for these environments.},
doi = {10.1145/1240624.1240701},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1145%2F1240624.1240701},
}
@InProceedings{Heer2009,
author = {Jeffrey Heer and Nicholas Kong and Maneesh Agrawala},
title = {Sizing the horizon},
booktitle = {Proceedings of the 27th international conference on Human factors in computing systems - {CHI} 09},
year = {2009},
publisher = {{ACM} Press},
abstract = {We investigate techniques for visualizing time series data and evaluate their effect in value comparison tasks. We compare line charts with horizon graphs - a space-efficient time series visualization technique - across a range of chart sizes, measuring the speed and accuracy of subjects' estimates of value differences between charts. We identify transition points at which reducing the chart height results in significantly differing drops in estimation accuracy across the compared chart types, and we find optimal positions in the speed-accuracy tradeoff curve at which viewers performed quickly without attendant drops in accuracy. Based on these results, we propose approaches for increasing data density that optimize graphical perception.},
doi = {10.1145/1518701.1518897},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1145%2F1518701.1518897},
}
@InProceedings{Heer2010,
author = {Jeffrey Heer and Michael Bostock},
title = {Crowdsourcing graphical perception},
booktitle = {Proceedings of the 28th international conference on Human factors in computing systems - {CHI} '10},
year = {2010},
publisher = {{ACM} Press},
abstract = {Understanding perception is critical to effective visualization design. With its low cost and scalability, crowdsourcing presents an attractive option for evaluating the large design space of visualizations; however, it first requires validation. In this paper, we assess the viability of Amazon's Mechanical Turk as a platform for graphical perception experiments. We replicate previous studies of spatial encoding and luminance contrast and compare our results. We also conduct new experiments on rectangular area perception (as in treemaps or cartograms) and on chart size and gridline spacing. Our results demonstrate that crowdsourced perception experiments are viable and contribute new insights for visualization design. Lastly, we report cost and performance data from our experiments and distill recommendations for the design of crowdsourced studies.},
doi = {10.1145/1753326.1753357},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1145%2F1753326.1753357},
keywords = {crowdsourcing}
}
@Article{Kong2010,
author = {N Kong and J Heer and M Agrawala},
title = {Perceptual Guidelines for Creating Rectangular Treemaps},
journal = {{IEEE} Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics},
year = {2010},
volume = {16},
number = {6},
pages = {990--998},
month = {nov},
abstract = {Treemaps aspace-fillingre space-filling visualizations that make efficient use of limited display space to depict large amounts of hierarchical data. Creating perceptually effective treemaps requires carefully managing a number of design parameters including the aspect ratio and luminance of rectangles. Moreover, treemaps encode values using area, which has been found to be less accurate than judgments of other visual encodings, such as length. We conduct a series of controlled experiments aimed at producing a set of design guidelines for creating effective rectangular treemaps. We find no evidence that luminance affects area judgments, but observe that aspect ratio does have an effect. Specifically, we find that the accuracy of area comparisons suffers when the compared rectangles have extreme aspect ratios or when both are squares. Contrary to common assumptions, the optimal distribution of rectangle aspect ratios within a treemap should include non-squares, but should avoid extreme aspect ratios. We then compare treemaps with hierarchical bar chart displays to identify the data densities at which length-encoded bar charts become less effective than area-encoded treemaps. We report the transition points at which treemaps exhibit judgment accuracy on par with bar charts for both leaf and non-leaf tree nodes. We also find that even at relatively low data densities treemaps result in faster comparisons than bar charts. Based on these results, we present a set of guidelines for the effective use of treemaps.},
doi = {10.1109/tvcg.2010.186},
publisher = {Institute of Electrical and Electronics Engineers ({IEEE})},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1109%2Ftvcg.2010.186},
}
@Article{Liu2014,
author = {Zhicheng Liu and Jeffrey Heer},
title = {The Effects of Interactive Latency on Exploratory Visual Analysis},
journal = {{IEEE} Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics},
year = {2014},
volume = {20},
number = {12},
pages = {2122--2131},
month = {dec},
abstract = {To support effective exploration, it is often stated that interactive visualizations should provide rapid response times. However, the effects of interactive latency on the process and outcomes of exploratory visual analysis have not been systematically studied. We present an experiment measuring user behavior and knowledge discovery with interactive visualizations under varying latency conditions. We observe that an additional delay of 500ms incurs significant costs, decreasing user activity and data set coverage. Analyzing verbal data from think-aloud protocols, we find that increased latency reduces the rate at which users make observations, draw generalizations and generate hypotheses. Moreover, we note interaction effects in which initial exposure to higher latencies leads to subsequently reduced performance in a low-latency setting. Overall, increased latency causes users to shift exploration strategy, in turn affecting performance. We discuss how these results can inform the design of interactive analysis tools.},
doi = {10.1109/tvcg.2014.2346452},
publisher = {Institute of Electrical and Electronics Engineers ({IEEE})},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1109%2Ftvcg.2014.2346452},
}
@Article{Kay2016,
author = {Matthew Kay and Jeffrey Heer},
title = {Beyond Weber's Law: A Second Look at Ranking Visualizations of Correlation},
journal = {{IEEE} Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics},
year = {2016},
volume = {22},
number = {1},
pages = {469--478},
month = {jan},
abstract = {Models of human perception - including perceptual “laws” - can be valuable tools for deriving visualization design recommendations. However, it is important to assess the explanatory power of such models when using them to inform design. We present a secondary analysis of data previously used to rank the effectiveness of bivariate visualizations for assessing correlation (measured with Pearson's r) according to the well-known Weber-Fechner Law. Beginning with the model of Harrison et al. [1], we present a sequence of refinements including incorporation of individual differences, log transformation, censored regression, and adoption of Bayesian statistics. Our model incorporates all observations dropped from the original analysis, including data near ceilings caused by the data collection process and entire visualizations dropped due to large numbers of observations worse than chance. This model deviates from Weber's Law, but provides improved predictive accuracy and generalization. Using Bayesian credibility intervals, we derive a partial ranking that groups visualizations with similar performance, and we give precise estimates of the difference in performance between these groups. We find that compared to other visualizations, scatterplots are unique in combining low variance between individuals and high precision on both positively- and negatively correlated data. We conclude with a discussion of the value of data sharing and replication, and share implications for modeling similar experimental data.},
doi = {10.1109/tvcg.2015.2467671},
keywords = {scatterplots},
publisher = {Institute of Electrical and Electronics Engineers ({IEEE})},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1109%2Ftvcg.2015.2467671},
}
@Article{Ziemkiewicz2013,
author = {C. Ziemkiewicz and A. Ottley and R. J. Crouser and A. R. Yauilla and S. L. Su and W. Ribarsky and R. Chang},
title = {How Visualization Layout Relates to Locus of Control and Other Personality Factors},
journal = {{IEEE} Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics},
year = {2013},
volume = {19},
number = {7},
pages = {1109--1121},
month = {jul},
abstract = {Existing research suggests that individual personality differences are correlated with a user’s speed and accuracy in solving problems with different types of complex visualization systems. We extend this research by isolating factors in personality traits as well as in the visualizations that could have contributed to the observed correlation. We focus on a personality trait known as "locus of control" (LOC), which represents a person’s tendency to see themselves as controlled by or in control of external events. To isolate variables of the visualization design, we control extraneous factors such as color, interaction, and labeling. We conduct a user study with four visualizations that gradually shift from a list metaphor to a containment metaphor and compare the participants’ speed, accuracy, and preference with their locus of control and other personality factors. Our findings demonstrate that there is indeed a correlation between the two: participants with an internal locus of control perform more poorly with visualizations that employ a containment metaphor, while those with an external locus of control perform well with such visualizations. These results provide evidence for the externalization theory of visualization. Finally, we propose applications of these findings to adaptive visual analytics and visualization evaluation.},
doi = {10.1109/tvcg.2012.180},
publisher = {Institute of Electrical and Electronics Engineers ({IEEE})},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1109%2Ftvcg.2012.180},
}
@InProceedings{Levy1996,
author = {Ellen Levy and Jeff Zacks and Barbara Tversky and Diane Schiano},
title = {Gratuitous graphics? Putting preferences in perspective},
booktitle = {Proceedings of the {SIGCHI} conference on Human factors in computing systems common ground - {CHI} '96},
year = {1996},
publisher = {{ACM} Press},
abstract = {Rapid growth in 3-D rendering technologies has deluged us with glitzy graphical representations. In what contexts do people find 3-D graphs of 2-D data both attractive and useful? We examine students' preferences for graphical display formats under several use scenarios. Line graphs were preferred more for conveying trends than details, and more for promoting memorability than for immediate use; bar graphs showed the opposite pattern. 3-D graphs were preferred more for depicting details than trends, more for memorability than immediate use, and more for showing others than oneself. The reverse held for 2-D graphs.},
doi = {10.1145/238386.238400},
keywords = {3D},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1145%2F238386.238400},
}
@Article{Spence1990,
author = {Ian Spence},
title = {Visual psychophysics of simple graphical elements.},
journal = {Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance},
year = {1990},
volume = {16},
number = {4},
pages = {683--692},
abstract = {The accuracy with which graphical elements are judged was assessed in a psychophysical task that parallels the real-life use of graphs. The task is a variant of the Metfessel-Comrey constant-sum method, and an associated model based on Stevens's law is proposed. The stimuli were horizontal and vertical lines, bars, pie and disk slices, cylinders, boxes, and table entries (numbers). Stevens's law exponents were near unity for numbers and 1-dimensional elements but were also close to 1 for elements possessing 2 or 3 apparent dimensions (Ss accommodate extraneous dimensions that do not carry variation, changing the effective dimensionality of the stimulus). Judgment errors were small, with numbers yielding the best performance; elements such as bars and pie slices were judged almost as accurately; disk elements were judged least accurately, but the magnitude of the errors was not large.},
doi = {10.1037/0096-1523.16.4.683},
publisher = {American Psychological Association ({APA})},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1037%2F0096-1523.16.4.683},
}
@Article{Schiano1992,
author = {Diane J. Schiano and Barbara Tversky},
title = {Structure and strategy in encoding simplified graphs},
journal = {Memory & Cognition},
year = {1992},
volume = {20},
number = {1},
pages = {12--20},
abstract = {Tversky and Schiano (1989) found a systematic bias toward the 45° line in memory for the slopes of identical lines when embedded in graphs, but not in maps, suggesting the use of a cognitive reference frame specifically for encoding meaningful graphs. The present experiments explore this issue further using the linear configurations alone as stimuli. Experimental and 2 demonstrate that perception and immediate memory for the slope of a test line within orthogonal “axes” are predictable from purely structural considerations. In Experiments 3 and 4, subjects were instructed to use a diagonal-reference strategy in viewing the stimuli, which were described as “graphs” only in Experiment 3. Results for both studies showed the diagonal bias previously found only for graphs. This pattern provides converging evidence for the diagonal as a cognitive reference frame in encoding linear graphs, and demonstrates that even in highly simplified displays, strategic factors can produce encoding biases not predictable solely from stimulus structure alone.},
month = {jan},
doi = {10.3758/bf03208249},
publisher = {Springer Nature},
url = {https://doi.org/10.3758%2Fbf03208249},
}
@Article{Zacks1999,
author = {Jeff Zacks and Barbara Tversky},
title = {Bars and lines: A study of graphic communication},
journal = {Memory & Cognition},
year = {1999},
volume = {27},
number = {6},
pages = {1073--1079},
month = {nov},
abstract = {Interpretations of graphs seem to be rooted in principles of cognitive naturalness and information processing rather than arbitrary correspondences. These predict that people should more readily associate bars with discrete comparisons between data points because bars are discrete entities and facilitate point estimates. They should more readily associate lines with trends because lines connect discrete entities and directly represent slope. The predictions were supported in three experiments—two examining comprehension and one production. The correspondence does not seem to depend on explicit knowledge of rules. Instead, it may reflect the influence of the communicative situation as well as the perceptual properties of graphs.},
doi = {10.3758/bf03201236},
publisher = {Springer Nature},
url = {https://doi.org/10.3758%2Fbf03201236},
}
@InProceedings{Harrison2013,
author = {Lane Harrison and Drew Skau and Steven Franconeri and Aidong Lu and Remco Chang},
title = {Influencing visual judgment through affective priming},
booktitle = {Proceedings of the {SIGCHI} Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems - {CHI} '13},
year = {2013},
publisher = {{ACM} Press},
abstract = {Recent research suggests that individual personality differences can influence performance with visualizations. In addition to stable personality traits, research in psychology has found that temporary changes in affect (emotion) can also significantly impact performance during cognitive tasks. In this paper, we show that affective priming also influences user performance on visual judgment tasks through an experiment that combines affective priming with longstanding graphical perception experiments. Our results suggest that affective priming can influence accuracy in common graphical perception tasks. We discuss possible explanations for these findings, and describe how these findings can be applied to design visualizations that are less (or more) susceptible to error in common visualization contexts.},
doi = {10.1145/2470654.2481410},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1145%2F2470654.2481410},
}
@Article{Cleveland1984,
author = {W S Cleveland and Robert McGill},
title = {Graphical Perception: Theory, Experimentation, and Application to the Development of Graphical Methods},
journal = {Journal of the American Statistical Association},
year = {1984},
volume = {79},
number = {387},
pages = {531--554},
abstract = {The subject of graphical methods for data analysis and for data presentation needs a scientific foundation. In this article we take a few steps in the direction of establishing such a foundation. Our approach is based on graphical perception—the visual decoding of information encoded on graphs—and it includes both theory and experimentation to test the theory. The theory deals with a small but important piece of the whole process of graphical perception. The first part is an identification of a set of elementary perceptual tasks that are carried out when people extract quantitative information from graphs. The second part is an ordering of the tasks on the basis of how accurately people perform them. Elements of the theory are tested by experimentation in which subjects record their judgments of the quantitative information on graphs. The experiments validate these elements but also suggest that the set of elementary tasks should be expanded. The theory provides a guideline for graph construction: Graphs should employ elementary tasks as high in the ordering as possible. This principle is applied to a variety of graphs, including bar charts, divided bar charts, pie charts, and statistical maps with shading. The conclusion is that radical surgery on these popular graphs is needed, and as replacements we offer alternative graphical forms—dot charts, dot charts with grouping, and framed-rectangle charts.},
doi = {10.2307/2288400},
}
@InProceedings{Cole2009,
author = {Forrester Cole and Kevin Sanik and Doug DeCarlo and Adam Finkelstein and Thomas Funkhouser and Szymon Rusinkiewicz and Manish Singh},
title = {How well do line drawings depict shape?},
booktitle = {{ACM} {SIGGRAPH} 2009 papers on - {SIGGRAPH} '09},
year = {2009},
publisher = {{ACM} Press},
abstract = {This paper investigates the ability of sparse line drawings to depict 3D shape. We perform a study in which people are shown an image of one of twelve 3D objects depicted with one of six styles and asked to orient a gauge to coincide with the surface normal at many positions on the object’s surface. The normal estimates are compared with each other and with ground truth data provided by a registered 3D surface model to analyze accuracy and precision. The paper describes the design decisions made in collecting a large data set (275,000 gauge measurements) and provides analysis to answer questions about how well people interpret shapes from drawings. Our findings suggest that people interpret certain shapes almost as well from a line drawing as from a shaded image, that current computer graphics line drawing techniques can effectively depict shape and even match the effectiveness of artist’s drawings, and that errors in depiction are often localized and can be traced to particular properties of the lines used. The data collected for this study will become a publicly available resource for further studies of this type.},
doi = {10.1145/1576246.1531334},
keywords = {3D},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1145%2F1576246.1531334},
}
@InProceedings{Haroz2015,
author = {Steve Haroz and Robert Kosara and Steven L. Franconeri},
title = {{ISOTYPE} Visualization},
booktitle = {Proceedings of the 33rd Annual {ACM} Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems - {CHI} '15},
year = {2015},
publisher = {{ACM} Press},
abstract = {Although the infographic and design communities have used simple pictographic representations for decades, it is still unclear whether they can make visualizations more effective. Using simple charts, we tested how pictographic representations impact (1) memory for information just viewed, as well as under the load of additional information, (2) speed of finding information, and (3) engagement and preference in seeking out these visualizations. We find that superfluous images can distract. But we find no user costs -- and some intriguing benefits -- when pictographs are used to represent the data.
},
doi = {10.1145/2702123.2702275},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1145%2F2702123.2702275},
}
@Article{Hollands1998,
author = {J. G. Hollands and Ian Spence},
title = {Judging proportion with graphs: the summation model},
journal = {Applied Cognitive Psychology},
year = {1998},
volume = {12},
number = {2},
pages = {173--190},
month = {apr},
abstract = {People take longer to judge part-to-whole relationships with bar graphs than with pie charts or divided bar graphs. Subjects may perform summation operations to establish the whole with bar graphs, which would be unnecessary for other graph types depicting the whole with a single object. To test this summation model, the number of components forming the whole was varied with bars, divided bars, reference bars, and pies in three experiments. Response time increased with the number of components for bar graphs but there was little increase for other graph types in Experiment 1. An accuracy emphasis in Experiment 2 produced generally longer response times, but had little effect on the time per summation. The summation operation was not used when graphs were displayed briefly in Experiment 3, although subjects still took longer with bars. The estimated time for a summation operation is consistent with estimates derived from other research. In general, the bar graph is not effective for proportion judgments, and its disadvantage becomes potentially greater as the number of components increases},
doi = {10.1002/(sici)1099-0720(199804)12:2<173::aid-acp499>3.0.co;2-k},
keywords = {pie-charts},
publisher = {Wiley-Blackwell},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1002%2F%28sici%291099-0720%28199804%2912%3A2%3C173%3A%3Aaid-acp499%3E3.0.co%3B2-k},
}
@InProceedings{Hullman2011,
author = {Jessica Hullman and Eytan Adar and Priti Shah},
title = {The impact of social information on visual judgments},
booktitle = {Proceedings of the 2011 annual conference on Human factors in computing systems - {CHI} '11},
year = {2011},
publisher = {{ACM} Press},
abstract = {Social visualization systems have emerged to support collective intelligence-driven analysis of a growing influx of open data. As with many other online systems, social signals (e.g., forums, polls) are commonly integrated to drive use. Unfortunately, the same social features that can provide rapid, high-accuracy analysis are coupled with the pitfalls of any social system. Through an experiment involving over 300 subjects, we address how social information signals (social proof) affect quantitative judgments in the context of graphical perception. We identify how unbiased social signals lead to fewer errors over non-social settings and conversely, how biased signals lead to more errors. We further reflect on how systematic bias nullifies certain collective intelligence benefits, and we provide evidence of the formation of information cascades. We describe how these findings can be applied to collaborative visualization systems to produce more accurate individual interpretations in social contexts.},
doi = {10.1145/1978942.1979157},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1145%2F1978942.1979157},
}
@Article{Simkin1987,
author = {David Simkin and Reid Hastie},
title = {An Information-Processing Analysis of Graph Perception},
journal = {Journal of the American Statistical Association},
year = {1987},
volume = {82},
number = {398},
pages = {454--465},
month = {jun},
abstract = {Social visualization systems have emerged to support collective intelligence-driven analysis of a growing influx of open data. As with many other online systems, social signals (e.g., forums, polls) are commonly integrated to drive use. Unfortunately, the same social features that can provide rapid, high-accuracy analysis are coupled with the pitfalls of any social system. Through an experiment involving over 300 subjects, we address how social information signals (social proof) affect quantitative judgments in the context of graphical perception. We identify how unbiased social signals lead to fewer errors over non-social settings and conversely, how biased signals lead to more errors. We further reflect on how systematic bias nullifies certain collective intelligence benefits, and we provide evidence of the formation of information cascades. We describe how these findings can be applied to collaborative visualization systems to produce more accurate individual interpretations in social contexts.},
doi = {10.1080/01621459.1987.10478448},
publisher = {Informa {UK} Limited},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1080%2F01621459.1987.10478448},
}
@Article{Robertson2008,
author = {G. Robertson and R. Fernandez and D. Fisher and B. Lee and J. Stasko},
title = {Effectiveness of Animation in Trend Visualization},
journal = {{IEEE} Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics},
year = {2008},
volume = {14},
number = {6},
pages = {1325--1332},
month = {nov},
abstract = {Animation has been used to show trends in multi-dimensional data. This technique has recently gained new prominence for presentations, most notably with Gapminder Trendalyzer. In Trendalyzer, animation together with interesting data and an engaging presenter helps the audience understand the results of an analysis of the data. It is less clear whether trend animation is effective for analysis. This paper proposes two alternative trend visualizations that use static depictions of trends: one which shows traces of all trends overlaid simultaneously in one display and a second that uses a small multiples display to show the trend traces side-by-side. The paper evaluates the three visualizations for both analysis and presentation. Results indicate that trend animation can be challenging to use even for presentations; while it is the fastest technique for presentation and participants find it enjoyable and exciting, it does lead to many participant errors. Animation is the least effective form for analysis; both static depictions of trends are significantly faster than animation, and the small multiples display is more accurate.},
doi = {10.1109/tvcg.2008.125},
keywords = {animation},
publisher = {Institute of Electrical and Electronics Engineers ({IEEE})},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1109%2Ftvcg.2008.125},
}
@Article{Harrison2014,
author = {Lane Harrison and Fumeng Yang and Steven Franconeri and Remco Chang},
title = {Ranking Visualizations of Correlation Using Weber's Law},
journal = {{IEEE} Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics},
year = {2014},
volume = {20},
number = {12},
pages = {1943--1952},
month = {dec},
abstract = {Despite years of research yielding systems and guidelines to aid visualization design, practitioners still face the challenge of identifying the best visualization for a given dataset and task. One promising approach to circumvent this problem is to leverage perceptual laws to quantitatively evaluate the effectiveness of a visualization design. Following previously established methodologies, we conduct a large scale (n=1687) crowdsourced experiment to investigate whether the perception of correlation in nine commonly used visualizations can be modeled using Weber’s law. The results of this experiment contribute to our understanding of information visualization by establishing that: 1) for all tested visualizations, the precision of correlation judgment could be modeled by Weber’s law, 2) correlation judgment precision showed striking variation between negatively and positively correlated data, and 3) Weber models provide a concise means to quantify, compare, and rank the perceptual precision afforded by a visualization.},
doi = {10.1109/tvcg.2014.2346979},
keywords = {scatterplots},
publisher = {Institute of Electrical and Electronics Engineers ({IEEE})},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1109%2Ftvcg.2014.2346979},
}
@InProceedings{Cawthon2007,
author = {Nick Cawthon and Andrew Vande Moere},
title = {The Effect of Aesthetic on the Usability of Data Visualization},
booktitle = {2007 11th International Conference Information Visualization ({IV} '07)},
year = {2007},
month = {jul},
publisher = {{IEEE}},
abstract = {Aesthetic seems currently under represented in most current data visualization evaluation methodologies. This paper investigates the results of an online survey of 285 participants, measuring both perceived aesthetic as well as the efficiency and effectiveness of retrieval tasks across a set of 11 different data visualization techniques. The data visualizations represent an identical hierarchical dataset, which has been normalized in terms of color, typography and layout balance. This study measured parameters such as speed of completion, accuracy rate, task abandonment and latency of erroneous response. Our findings demonstrate a correlation between latency in task abandonment and erroneous response time in relation to visualization's perceived aesthetic. These results support the need for an increased recognition for aesthetic in the typical evaluation process of data visualization techniques.},
doi = {10.1109/iv.2007.147},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1109%2Fiv.2007.147},
}
@Article{Lewandowsky1989,
author = {Stephan Lewandowsky and Ian Spence},
title = {Discriminating Strata in Scatterplots},
journal = {Journal of the American Statistical Association},
year = {1989},
volume = {84},
number = {407},
pages = {682--688},
month = {sep},
abstract = {When multiple groups are shown in a scatterplot each stratum is represented by a different symbol; for example, three strata might be coded using red, green, and yellow circles. Various symbol types were compared by behavioral experiment: Subjects were fastest when strata were coded using different colors and slowest when strata were coded with confusable letters—but there were no differences in accuracy. Accuracy differed only when processing time was restricted, again with different colors and confusable letters representing the two extremes. We conclude that color is the optimal symbol type and show that measuring response latency in addition to accuracy is essential in research on graphical perception.},
doi = {10.1080/01621459.1989.10478821},
keywords = {scatterplots},
publisher = {Informa {UK} Limited},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1080%2F01621459.1989.10478821},
}
@Article{Siegrist1996,
author = {Michael Siegrist},
title = {The use or misuse of three-dimensional graphs to represent lower-dimensional data},
journal = {Behaviour & Information Technology},
year = {1996},
volume = {15},
number = {2},
pages = {96--100},
month = {jan},
abstract = {Some statisticians hold strong opinions regarding graphs with a 3-D look. However, in experiments little attention has been paid to the effects of adding decorative depth. The performance of subjects on pie charts and bar charts with and without 3-D was evaluated in the present experiment. Subjects were asked to make relative magnitude estimates for different graphs. For pie charts, better performance was observed for 2-D than for 3-D charts. For the bar charts, a more differentiated picture emerged: performance was dependent on the position, height and dimension of the bars. However, 3-D bar charts had the one disadvantage that subjects needed more time to evaluate this type of graph.},
doi = {10.1080/014492996120300},
keywords = {pie-charts, 3D},
publisher = {Informa {UK} Limited},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1080%2F014492996120300},
}
@Article{Demiralp2014,
author = {Cagatay Demiralp Demiralp and Michael S. Bernstein and Jeffrey Heer},
title = {Learning Perceptual Kernels for Visualization Design},
journal = {{IEEE} Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics},
year = {2014},
volume = {20},
number = {12},
pages = {1933--1942},
month = {dec},
abstract = {Visualization design can benefit from careful consideration of perception, as different assignments of visual encoding variables such as color, shape and size affect how viewers interpret data. In this work, we introduce perceptual kernels: distance matrices derived from aggregate perceptual judgments. Perceptual kernels represent perceptual differences between and within visual variables in a reusable form that is directly applicable to visualization evaluation and automated design. We report results from crowd-sourced experiments to estimate kernels for color, shape, size and combinations thereof. We analyze kernels estimated using five different judgment types-including Likert ratings among pairs, ordinal triplet comparisons, and manual spatial arrangement-and compare them to existing perceptual models. We derive recommendations for collecting perceptual similarities, and then demonstrate how the resulting kernels can be applied to automate visualization design decisions.},
doi = {10.1109/tvcg.2014.2346978},
publisher = {Institute of Electrical and Electronics Engineers ({IEEE})},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1109%2Ftvcg.2014.2346978},
}
@Article{Cleveland1982,
author = {William S. Cleveland and Persi Diaconis and Robert McGill},
title = {Variables on Scatterplots Look More Highly Correlated When the Scales Are Increased},
journal = {Science},
year = {1982},
volume = {216},
number = {4550},
pages = {1138--1141},
month = {jun},
abstract = {Judged association between two variables represented on scatterplots increased when the scales on the horizontal and vertical axes were simultaneously increased so that the size of the point cloud within the frame of the plot decreased. Judged association was very diferentfrom the correlation coeficient, r, which is the most widely used measure of association.},
doi = {10.1126/science.216.4550.1138},
keywords = {scatterplots},
publisher = {American Association for the Advancement of Science ({AAAS})},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1126%2Fscience.216.4550.1138},
}
@Article{Spence2004,
author = {Ian Spence},
title = {The Apparent and Effective Dimensionality of Representations of Objects},
journal = {Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society},
year = {2004},
volume = {46},
number = {4},
pages = {738--747},
abstract = {Information displays commonly use 2-D and 3-D objects even though the numbers represented are 1-D. This practice may be problematic because the psychophysical relation between perceived and physical magnitudes is generally nonlinear for areas and volumes. Nonetheless, this research shows that apparent 2-D and 3-D objects can produce linear psychophysical functions if only one dimension shows variation. Processing time increases with the number of dimensions in the objects that show variation, not with the apparent dimensionality. Indeed, when only one dimension showed variation, apparent 3-D objects were judged more quickly than were apparent 2-D or 1-D objects. These results present a challenge for computational models of size perception and have implications for the design of information displays. Actual or potential applications of this research include the design and use of statistical graphs and information displays; objects that display variation in more than one dimension should not be used to represent single (1-D) numerical variables if they are to be judged accurately and rapidly.},
doi = {10.1518/hfes.46.4.738.56809},
keywords = {3D},
publisher = {{SAGE} Publications},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1518%2Fhfes.46.4.738.56809},
}
@InProceedings{Hale2017,
author = {Scott A. Hale and Graham McNeill and Jonathan Bright},
title = {Where'd it go? How geographic and force-directed layouts affect network task performances},
booktitle = {Eurographics Conference on Visualization (EuroVis) 2017 Volume 36 (2017), Number 3 J. Heer, T. Ropinski and J. van Wijk (Guest Editors)},
year = {2017},
abstract = {When visualizing geospatial network data, it is possible to position nodes according to their geographic locations or to po- sition nodes using standard network layout algorithms that ignore geographic location. Such data is increasingly common in interactive displays of Internet-connected sensor data, but network layouts that ignore geographic location data are rarely em- ployed. We conduct a user experiment to compare the effects of geographic and force-directed network layouts on three common network tasks: locating a node, determining the path length between two nodes, and comparing the degree of two nodes. We found a geographic layout was superior for locating a node but inferior for determining the path length between two nodes. The two layouts performed similarly when participants compared the degree of two nodes. We also tested a relaxed- or pseudo- geographic layout created with multidimensional scaling and found it performed as well or better than the pure geographic layout on all tasks but remained inferior to the force-directed layout for the path-length task. We suggest interactive displays of geospatial network data allow viewers to switch between geographic and force-directed layouts, although further research is needed to understand the extent to which viewers are able to choose the most appropriate layout for a given task.},
}
@InProceedings{Ziemkiewicz2010,
author = {Caroline Ziemkiewicz and Robert Kosara},
title = {Laws of Attraction: From Perceived Forces to Conceptual Similarity},
year = {2010},
publisher = {IEEE},
abstract = {Many of the pressing questions in information visualization deal with how exactly a user reads a collection of visual marks as information about relationships between entities. Previous research has suggested that people see parts of a visualization as objects, and may metaphorically interpret apparent physical relationships between these objects as suggestive of data relationships. We explored this hypothesis in detail in a series of user experiments. Inspired by the concept of implied dynamics in psychology, we first studied whether perceived gravity acting on a mark in a scatterplot can lead to errors in a participant’s recall of the mark’s position. The results of this study suggested that such position errors exist, but may be more strongly influenced by attraction between marks. We hypothesized that such apparent attraction may be influenced by elements used to suggest relationship between objects, such as connecting lines, grouping elements, and visual similarity. We further studied what visual elements are most likely to cause this attraction effect, and whether the elements that best predicted attraction errors were also those which suggested conceptual relationships most strongly. Our findings show a correlation between attraction errors and intuitions about relatedness, pointing towards a possible mechanism by which the perception of visual marks becomes an interpretation of data relationships.},
}
@InProceedings{Ziemkiewicz2010a,
author = {Caroline Ziemkiewicz and Robert Kosara},
title = {Implied dynamics in information visualization},
booktitle = {Proceedings of the International Conference on Advanced Visual Interfaces - {AVI} '10},
year = {2010},
publisher = {{ACM} Press},
abstract = {Information visualization is a powerful method for understanding and working with data. However, we still have an incomplete understanding of how people use visualization to think about information. We propose that people use visualization to support comprehension and reasoning by viewing abstract visual representations as physical scenes with a set of implied dynamics between objects. Inferences based on these implied dynamics are metaphorically extended to form inferences about the represented information. This view predicts that even seemingly meaningless properties of a visualization, including such minor design elements as borders, background areas, and the connectedness of parts, may affect how people perceive semantic aspects of data by suggesting different potential dynamics between data points. We present a study that supports this claim and discuss the design implications of this theory of information visualization.},
doi = {10.1145/1842993.1843031},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1145%2F1842993.1843031},
}
@InProceedings{Skau2017,
author = {Drew Skau and Robert Kosara},
title = {Readability and Precision in Pictorial Bar Charts},
booktitle = {Eurographics Conference on Visualization (EuroVis) 2017 Short Paper B. Kozlíková, T. Schreck, and T. Wischgoll (Editors)},
year = {2017},
abstract = {Bar charts embellished with unique artistic styles, or made to look like real objects, are common in information graphics. Embellishments are typically considered detrimental to readability and accuracy, since they add clutter and noise. Previous work has found that some of the shapes used, like rounded tops, triangles, etc., decreased accuracy when judging relative and absolute sizes, while T-shaped bars even showed a slight increase relative to the basic bar chart. In this paper, we report on a study that adds pictorial elements to bar charts of four different shapes tested previously, thus also including the elements of color and texture. We find that pictorial bar charts reduce accuracy, but not beyond the effect already observed for their shape. They also do not significantly increase response time. Embellished bar charts may not be as problematic as commonly assumed.},
}
@Article{Skau2016,
author = {Drew Skau and Robert Kosara},
title = {Arcs, Angles, or Areas: Individual Data Encodings in Pie and Donut Charts},
journal = {Computer Graphics Forum},
year = {2016},
volume = {35},
number = {3},
pages = {121--130},
month = {jun},
abstract = {Pie and donut charts have been a hotly debated topic in the visualization community for some time now. Even though pie charts have been around for over 200 years, our understanding of the perceptual factors used to read data in them is still limited. Data is encoded in pie and donut charts in three ways: arc length, center angle, and segment area. For our first study, we designed variations of pie charts to test the importance of individual encodings for reading accuracy. In our second study, we varied the inner radius of a donut chart from a filled pie to a thin outline to test the impact of removing the central angle. Both studies point to angle being the least important visual cue for both charts, and the donut chart being as accurate as the traditional pie chart.},
doi = {10.1111/cgf.12888},
keywords = {pie-charts},
publisher = {Wiley-Blackwell},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1111%2Fcgf.12888},
}
@InProceedings{Kosara2016,
author = {Robert Kosara and and Drew Skau},
title = {Judgment Error in Pie Chart Variations},
booktitle = {Eurographics Conference on Visualization (EuroVis) 2016 Short Paper E. Bertini, N. Elmqvist, and T. Wischgoll (Guest Editors)},
year = {2016},
abstract = {Pie charts and their variants are prevalent in business settings and many other uses, even if they are not popular with the academic community. In a recent study, we found that contrary to general belief, there is no clear evidence that these charts are read based on the central angle. Instead, area and arc length appear to be at least equally important. In this paper, we build on that study to test several pie chart variations that are popular in information graphics: exploded pie chart, pie with larger slice, elliptical pie, and square pie (in addition to a regular pie chart used as the baseline). We find that even variants that do not distort central angle cause greater error than regular pie charts. Charts that distort the shape show the highest error. Many of our predictions based on the previous study’s results are borne out by this study’s findings.},
doi = {10.2312/eurovisshort.20161167},
keywords = {pie-charts},
}
@InProceedings{Kosara2016a,
author = {Robert Kosara},
title = {An Empire Built On Sand},
booktitle = {Proceedings of the Beyond Time and Errors on Novel Evaluation Methods for Visualization - {BELIV} '16},
year = {2016},
publisher = {{ACM} Press},
abstract = {If we were to design Information Visualization from scratch, we would start with the basics: understand the principles of perception, test how they apply to different data encodings, build up those encodings to see if the principles still apply, etc. Instead, visualization was created from the other end: by building visual displays without an idea of how or if they worked, and then finding the relevant perceptual and other basics here and there.
This approach has the problem that we end up with a very patchy understanding of the foundations of our field. More than that, there is a good amount of unproven assumptions, aesthetic judgments, etc. mixed in with the evidence. We often don't even realize how much we rely on the latter, and can't easily identify them because they have been so deeply incorporated into the fabric of our field.
In this paper, I attempt to tease apart what we know and what we only think we know, using a few examples. The goal is to point out specific gaps in our knowledge, and to encourage researchers in the field to start questioning the underlying assumptions. Some of them are probably sound and will hold up to scrutiny. But some of them will not. We need to find out which is which and systematically build up a better foundation for our field. If we intend to develop ever more and better techniques and systems, we can't keep ignoring the base, or it will all come tumbling down sooner or later.},
doi = {10.1145/2993901.2993909},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1145%2F2993901.2993909},
}
@InProceedings{Kosara2010,
author = {Robert Kosara and Caroline Ziemkiewicz},
title = {Do Mechanical Turks dream of square pie charts?},
booktitle = {Proceedings of the 3rd {BELIV}'10 Workshop on {BEyond} time and errors: novel {evaLuation} methods for Information Visualization - {BELIV} '10},
year = {2010},
publisher = {{ACM} Press},
abstract = {Online studies are an attractive alternative to the laborintensive lab study, and promise the possibility of reaching a larger variety and number of people than at a typical university. There are also a number of draw-backs, however, that have made these studies largely impractical so far. Amazon's Mechanical Turk is a web service that facilitates the assignment of small, web-based tasks to a large pool of anonymous workers. We used it to conduct several perception and cognition studies, one of which was identical to a previous study performed in our lab. We report on our experiences and present ways to avoid common problems by taking them into account in the study design, and taking advantage of Mechanical Turk's features.},
doi = {10.1145/2110192.2110202},
keywords = {pie-charts},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1145%2F2110192.2110202},
}
@InProceedings{Hullman2017,
author = {Jessica Hullman and Robert Kosara and and Heidi Lam},
title = {Finding a Clear Path: Structuring Strategies for Visualization Sequences},
booktitle = {Eurographics Conference on Visualization (EuroVis) 2017 Volume 36 (2017), Number 3 J. Heer, T. Ropinski and J. van Wijk (Guest Editors)},
year = {2017},
abstract = {Little is known about how people structure sets of visualizations to support sequential viewing. We contribute findings from several studies examining visualization sequencing and reception. In our first study, people made decisions between various possible structures as they ordered a set of related visualizations (consisting of either bar charts or thematic maps) into what they considered the clearest sequence for showing the data. We find that most people structure visualization sequences hierarchically: they create high level groupings based on shared data properties like time period, measure, level of aggregation, and spatial region, then order the views within these groupings. We also observe a tendency for certain types of similarities between views, like a common spatial region or aggregation level, to be seen as more appropriate categories for organizing views in a sequence than others, like a common time period or measure. In a second study, we find that viewers’ perceptions of the quality and intention of different sequences are largely consistent with the perceptions of the users who created them. The understanding of sequence preferences and perceptions that emerges from our studies has implications for the development of visualization authoring tools and sequence recommendations for guided analysis.},
}
@Article{Gattis1996,
author = {Merideth Gattis and Keith J. Holyoak},
title = {Mapping conceptual to spatial relations in visual reasoning.},
journal = {Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition},
year = {1996},
volume = {22},
number = {1},
pages = {231--239},
abstract = {In 3 experiments, the authors investigated the impact of goals and perceptual relations on graph interpretation when people evaluate functional dependencies between continuous variables. Participants made inferences about the relative rate of 2 continuous linear variables (altitude and temperature). The authors varied the assignments of variables to axes, the perceived cause–effect relation between the variables, and the causal status of the variable being queried. The most striking finding was that accuracy was greater when the slope-mapping constraint was honored, which requires that the variable being queried be assigned to the vertical axis, so that steeper lines map to faster changes in the queried variable. The authors propose that graphs provide external instantiations of intermediate mental representations, enabling people to move from visuospatial representations to abstractions through the use of natural mappings between perceptual and conceptual relations.},
doi = {10.1037/0278-7393.22.1.231},
publisher = {American Psychological Association ({APA})},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1037%2F0278-7393.22.1.231},
}
@Article{Elting1999,
author = {L. S Elting and C. G Martin and S. B Cantor and E. B Rubenstein},
title = {Influence of data display formats on physician investigators' decisions to stop clinical trials: prospective trial with repeated measures},
journal = {{BMJ}},
year = {1999},
volume = {318},
number = {7197},
pages = {1527--1531},
month = {jun},
abstract = {Objective: To examine the effect of the method of data display on physician investigators' decisions to stop hypothetical clinical trials for an unplanned statistical analysis.
Design: Prospective, mixed model design with variables between subjects and within subjects (repeated measures).
Setting: Comprehensive cancer centre.
Participants: 34 physicians, stratified by academic rank, who were conducting clinical trials.
Interventions:Participants were shown tables, pie charts, bar graphs, and icon displays containing hypothetical data from a clinical trial and were asked to decide whether to continue the trial or stop for an unplanned statistical analysis.
Main outcome measure:Percentage of accurate decisions with each type of display.
Results: Accuracy of decisions was affected by the type of data display and positive or negative framing of the data. More correct decisions were made with icon displays than with tables, pie charts, and bar graphs (82% v 68%, 56%, and 43%, respectively; P=0.03) and when data were negatively framed rather than positively framed in tables (93% v 47%; P=0.004).
Conclusions: Clinical investigators' decisions can be affected by factors unrelated to the actual data. In the design of clinical trials information systems, careful consideration should be given to the method by which data are framed and displayed in order to reduce the impact of these extraneous factors.},
doi = {10.1136/bmj.318.7197.1527},
publisher = {{BMJ}},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1136%2Fbmj.318.7197.1527},
}
@Article{Rensink2010,
author = {Ronald A. Rensink and Gideon Baldridge},
title = {The Perception of Correlation in Scatterplots},
journal = {Computer Graphics Forum},
year = {2010},
volume = {29},
number = {3},
pages = {1203--1210},
month = {aug},
abstract = {We present a rigorous way to evaluate the visual perception of correlation in scatterplots, based on classical psychophysical methods originally developed for simple properties such as brightness. Although scatterplots are graphically complex, the quantity they convey is relatively simple. As such, it may be possible to assess the perception of correlation in a similar way.
Scatterplots were each of 5.0° extent, containing 100 points with a bivariate normal distribution. Means were 0.5 of the range of the points, and standard deviations 0.2 of this range. Precision was determined via an adaptive algorithm to find the just noticeable differences (jnds) in correlation, i.e., the difference between two side-by-side scatterplots that could be discriminated 75% of the time. Accuracy was measured by direct estimation, using reference scatterplots with fixed upper and lower values, with a test scatterplot adjusted so that its correlation appeared to be halfway between these. This process was recursively applied to yield several further estimates.
Results of the discrimination tests show jnd(r) = k (1/b – r), where r is the Pearson correlation, and parameters 0 < k, b < 1. Integration yields a subjective estimate of correlation g(r) = ln(1 – br) / ln(1 – b). The values of b found via discrimination closely match those found via direct estimation. As such, it appears that the perception of correlation in a scatterplot is completely described by two related performance curves, specified by two easily-measured parameters.},
doi = {10.1111/j.1467-8659.2009.01694.x},
keywords = {scatterplots},
publisher = {Wiley-Blackwell},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1111%2Fj.1467-8659.2009.01694.x},
}
@Article{Croxton1927,
author = {Frederick E. Croxton and Roy E. Stryker},
title = {Bar Charts versus Circle Diagrams},
journal = {Journal of the American Statistical Association},
year = {1927},
volume = {22},
number = {160},
pages = {473--482},
month = {dec},
doi = {10.1080/01621459.1927.10502976},
keywords = {pie-charts},
publisher = {Informa {UK} Limited},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1080%2F01621459.1927.10502976},
}
@Article{Eells1926,
author = {Walter Crosby Eells},
title = {The Relative Merits of Circles and Bars for Representing Component Parts},
journal = {Journal of the American Statistical Association},
year = {1926},
volume = {21},
number = {154},
pages = {119--132},
month = {jun},
doi = {10.1080/01621459.1926.10502165},
keywords = {pie-charts},
publisher = {Informa {UK} Limited},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1080%2F01621459.1926.10502165},
}
@Article{Croxton1932,
author = {Frederick E. Croxton and Harold Stein},
title = {Graphic Comparisons by Bars, Squares, Circles, and Cubes},
journal = {Journal of the American Statistical Association},
year = {1932},
volume = {27},
number = {177},
pages = {54--60},
month = {mar},
doi = {10.1080/01621459.1932.10503227},
keywords = {pie-charts, 3D},
publisher = {Informa {UK} Limited},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1080%2F01621459.1932.10503227},
}
@Article{Huhn1927,
author = {R. von Huhn},
title = {Further Studies in the Graphic Use of Circles and Bars},
journal = {Journal of the American Statistical Association},
year = {1927},
volume = {22},
number = {157},
pages = {31--36},
month = {mar},
doi = {10.1080/01621459.1927.10502938},
keywords = {pie-charts},
publisher = {Informa {UK} Limited},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1080%2F01621459.1927.10502938},
}
@Article{Spence1991,
author = {Ian Spence and Stephan Lewandowsky},
title = {Displaying proportions and percentages},
journal = {Applied Cognitive Psychology},
year = {1991},
volume = {5},
number = {1},
pages = {61--77},
month = {jan},
abstract = {Pie and bar charts are commonly used to display percentage or proportional data, but professional data analysts have frowned on the use of the pie chart on the grounds that judgements of area are less accurate than judgements of lenth. Thus the bar chart has been favoured. When the amount of data to be communicated is small, some authorities have advocated the use of properly constructed tables, as another option. The series of experiments reported here suggests that there is little to choose between the pie and the bar chart, with the former enjoying a slight advantage if the required judgement is a complicated one, but that both forms of chart are superior to the table. Thus our results do not support the commonly expressed opinion that pie charts are inferior. An analysis of the nature of the task and a review of the psychophysical literature suggest that the traditional prejudice against the pie chart is misguided.},
doi = {10.1002/acp.2350050106},
keywords = {pie-charts},
publisher = {Wiley-Blackwell},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1002%2Facp.2350050106},
}
@Article{Flannery1971,
author = {James John Flannery},
title = {The relative effectiveness of some common graduated point symbols in the presentation of quantitative data},
journal = {Cartographica: The International Journal for Geographic Information and Geovisualization},
year = {1971},
volume = {8},
number = {2},
pages = {96--109},
month = {dec},
abstract = {Circles with their areas varying in direct proportion to quantities represented are a common form of graduated point symbols. When so used, unfortunately, the average map reader perceives a smaller quantitative difference than intended because circle size differences are usually underestimated. An apparent size scale developed empirically fifteen years ago is claimed to eliminate the problem of consistent underestimation. More recent investigations by psychologists and cartographers support the apparent size scale. Bars communicate quantitative variation effectively when graduated in the traditional manner on a linear basis, but wedges require an apparent size scale and even then are less accurately judged.},
doi = {10.3138/j647-1776-745h-3667},
publisher = {University of Toronto Press Inc. ({UTPress})},
url = {https://doi.org/10.3138%2Fj647-1776-745h-3667},
}
@Article{Lee2016,
author = {Sukwon Lee and Sung-Hee Kim and Ya-Hsin Hung and Heidi Lam and Youn-Ah Kang and Ji Soo Yi},
title = {How do People Make Sense of Unfamiliar Visualizations?: A Grounded Model of Novice's Information Visualization Sensemaking},
journal = {{IEEE} Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics},
year = {2016},
volume = {22},
number = {1},
pages = {499--508},
month = {jan},
abstract = {In this paper, we would like to investigate how people make sense of unfamiliar information visualizations. In order to achieve the research goal, we conducted a qualitative study by observing 13 participants when they endeavored to make sense of three unfamiliar visualizations (i.e., a parallel-coordinates plot, a chord diagram, and a treemap) that they encountered for the first time. We collected data including audio/video record of think-aloud sessions and semi-structured interview; and analyzed the data using the grounded theory method. The primary result of this study is a grounded model of NOvice's information VIsualization Sensemaking (NOVIS model), which consists of the five major cognitive activities: 1 encountering visualization, 2 constructing a frame, 3 exploring visualization, 4 questioning the frame, and 5 floundering on visualization. We introduce the NOVIS model by explaining the five activities with representative quotes from our participants. We also explore the dynamics in the model. Lastly, we compare with other existing models and share further research directions that arose from our observations.},
doi = {10.1109/tvcg.2015.2467195},
keywords = {parallel-coordinates},
publisher = {Institute of Electrical and Electronics Engineers ({IEEE})},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1109%2Ftvcg.2015.2467195},
}
@Article{Carswell1993,
author = {C. Melody Carswell and Cathy Emery and Andrea M. Lonon},
title = {Stimulus complexity and information integration in the spontaneous interpretations of line graphs},
journal = {Applied Cognitive Psychology},
year = {1993},
volume = {7},
number = {4},
pages = {341--357},
month = {aug},
abstract = {Viewers of a graph will readily interpret its contents, even when given no explicit instructions regarding what information to extract. However, little is known about the strategies that subjects adopt when making such spontaneous interpretations. In the present experiments, subjects studied single-function line graphs for self-determined periods. They provided written interpretations immediately following examination of each graph. The structural complexity of stimulus graphs was varied by eliminating symmetry, and by adding data points, departures from linearity, and trend reversals. Across two experiments, number of trend reversals was the main determinant of comprehension difficulty as measured by study times. An increased number of reversals also resulted in more local, detail-oriented content in interpretations. By contrast, the presence of such emergent features as symmetry and linearity led to increases in the amount of integrative, global content in interpretations, usually at the expense of local detail. Surprisingly, increases in the number of data points led to similar increases in the grain of subjects' interpretations. The last finding may reflect a shift from point-by-point to integrative study strategies necessitated by capacity limitations in working memory.},
doi = {10.1002/acp.2350070407},
publisher = {Wiley-Blackwell},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1002%2Facp.2350070407},
}
@Article{Hollands1992,
author = {J. G. Hollands and Ian Spence},
title = {Judgments of change and proportion in graphical perception.},
journal = {J. G. Hollands and Ian Spence Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society},
year = {1992},
volume = {34},
number = {3},
pages = {313-334},
abstract = {Subjects judged change and proportion when viewing graphs in two experiments. Change was judged more quickly and accurately with line and bar graphs than with pie charts or tiered bar graphs, and this difference was larger when the rate of change was smaller. Without a graduated scale, proportion was judged more quickly and accurately with pie charts and divided bar graphs than with line or bar graphs. Perception is direct when it requires simpler or fewer mental operations; we propose that perception of change is direct with line and bar graphs, whereas perception of proportion is direct with pie charts and divided bar graphs. The results are also consistent with the proximity compatibility principle. Suggestions for improving the design of graphical displays are given.},
keywords = {pie-charts},
}
@Article{Shah2011,
author = {Shah, Priti and Freedman, Eric G.},
title = {Bar and Line Graph Comprehension: An Interaction of Top-Down and Bottom-Up Processes},
journal = {Topics in Cognitive Science},
year = {2011},
volume = {3},
number = {3},
pages = {560--578},
issn = {1756-8765},
abstract = {This experiment investigated the effect of format (line vs. bar), viewers’ familiarity with variables, and viewers’ graphicacy (graphical literacy) skills on the comprehension of multivariate (three variable) data presented in graphs. Fifty-five undergraduates provided written descriptions of data for a set of 14 line or bar graphs, half of which depicted variables familiar to the population and half of which depicted variables unfamiliar to the population. Participants then took a test of graphicacy skills. As predicted, the format influenced viewers’ interpretations of data. Specifically, viewers were more likely to describe x–y interactions when viewing line graphs than when viewing bar graphs, and they were more likely to describe main effects and “z–y” (the variable in the legend) interactions when viewing bar graphs than when viewing line graphs. Familiarity of data presented and individuals’ graphicacy skills interacted with the influence of graph format. Specifically, viewers were most likely to generate inferences only when they had high graphicacy skills, the data were familiar and thus the information inferred was expected, and the format supported those inferences. Implications for multivariate data display are discussed.},
doi = {10.1111/j.1756-8765.2009.01066.x},
publisher = {Blackwell Publishing Ltd},
url = {http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1756-8765.2009.01066.x},
}
@Article{Correll2014,
author = {Michael Correll and Michael Gleicher},
title = {Error Bars Considered Harmful: Exploring Alternate Encodings for Mean and Error},
journal = {{IEEE} Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics},
year = {2014},
volume = {20},
number = {12},
pages = {2142--2151},
month = {dec},
abstract = {When making an inference or comparison with uncertain, noisy, or incomplete data, measurement error and confidence intervals can be as important for judgment as the actual mean values of different groups. These often misunderstood statistical quantities are frequently represented by bar charts with error bars. This paper investigates drawbacks with this standard encoding, and considers a set of alternatives designed to more effectively communicate the implications of mean and error data to a general audience, drawing from lessons learned from the use of visual statistics in the information visualization community. We present a series of crowd-sourced experiments that confirm that the encoding of mean and error significantly changes how viewers make decisions about uncertain data. Careful consideration of design tradeoffs in the visual presentation of data results in human reasoning that is more consistently aligned with statistical inferences. We suggest the use of gradient plots (which use transparency to encode uncertainty) and violin plots (which use width) as better alternatives for inferential tasks than bar charts with error bars.},
doi = {10.1109/tvcg.2014.2346298},
publisher = {Institute of Electrical and Electronics Engineers ({IEEE})},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1109%2Ftvcg.2014.2346298},
}
@Article{Bobko1979,
author = {Philip Bobko and Ronald Karren},
title = {The perception of Pearson product moment correlations from bivariate scatterplots},
journal = {Personnel Psychology},
year = {1979},
volume = {32},
number = {2},
pages = {313--325},
month = {jun},
abstract = {Perceptions about the Pearson product moment correlation, r, from bivariate scatterplots were investigated through the use of a questionnaire. It was found that subjects who are relatively sophisticated in psychometric techniques tend to underestimate the magnitude of r, with most pronounced disparity in the range .2 < |r| < .6. Additionally, estimates of r from specially designed scatterplots indicated that subjects (1) correctly estimated the effects of range restriction, (2) underestimated the effects of attenuating outliers, (3) incorrectly reduced estimates of r when the regression slope was relatively high or low, and (4) often failed to consider the effects of removing the middle third of the data. Several implications of these generally conservative estimations are discussed.},
doi = {10.1111/j.1744-6570.1979.tb02137.x},
keywords = {scatterplots},
publisher = {Wiley-Blackwell},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1111%2Fj.1744-6570.1979.tb02137.x},
}
@Article{Doherty2007,
author = {Michael E. Doherty and Richard B. Anderson and Andrea M. Angott and Dale S. Klopfer},
title = {The perception of scatterplots},
journal = {Perception & Psychophysics},
year = {2007},
volume = {69},
number = {7},
pages = {1261--1272},
month = {oct},
abstract = {Four experiments investigated the perception of correlations from scatterplots. All graphic properties, other than error variance, that have been shown to affect subjective but not objective correlation(r) were held constant. Participants in Experiment 1 ranked 21 scatterplots according to the magnitude ofr. In Experiments 2 and 3, participants made yes/no judgments to indicate whether a scatterplot was high (signal) or low (noise). Values ofr for signal and noise scatterplots varied across participants. Differences between correlations for signal and for noise scatterplots were constant inr in Experiment 2, and constant inr2 in Experiment 3. Standard deviations of the ranks in Experiment 1 and ď values in Experiments 2 and 3 showed that discriminability increased with the magnitude ofr. In Experiment 4, faculty and graduate students in psychology and sociology made point estimates ofr for single scatterplots. Estimates were negatively accelerated functions of objective correlation.},
doi = {10.3758/bf03193961},
keywords = {scatterplots},
publisher = {Springer Nature},
url = {https://doi.org/10.3758%2Fbf03193961},
}
@Article{Lauer1989,
author = {Thomas W. Lauer and Gerald V. Post},
title = {Density in scatterplots and the estimation of correlation},
journal = {Behaviour & Information Technology},
year = {1989},
volume = {8},
number = {3},
pages = {235--244},
abstract = {The construction of a graphical presentation involves the representation of information by means of visual symbols.The acquisition of information from the resultant graph is a perceptual process that involves the decoding and interpretation of the visual symbols. Hence good design decisions will be based on an understanding of the information acquisition process and in particular graphical perception. This study examines the perception of bivariate normal data presented in a scatter diagram, and creates a model that successfully explains how individuals perceive the information contained in scatterplots. Subjects were shown a series of scatter diagrams on the CRT of a microcomputer and were asked to estimate correlation. Several variables were examined that explain estimated correlation including regression slope, dispersion, number of points displayed, and the size of the CRT screen. All of these factors were found to significantly affect subjects' estimates of correlation.},
doi = {10.1080/01449298908914554},
keywords = {scatterplots},
publisher = {Informa {UK} Limited},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1080%2F01449298908914554},
}
@Article{Meyer1997,
author = {Joachim Meyer and Meirav Taieb and Ittai Flascher},
title = {Correlation estimates as perceptual judgments.},
journal = {Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied},
year = {1997},
volume = {3},
number = {1},
pages = {3--20},
abstract = { Correlation estimates from scatterplots were studied as an example for an intuitive decision task. Three experiments showed that subjective correlation estimates are based on geometric properties of the displays. People with different levels of statistical training were found to assess correlations from scatterplots in close accordance with the power function rest = 1 – aXb, where X is the mean of the geometrical distances between the points and the regression line or a similar central axis. Changes of the slope of the displayed point cloud and the introduction of outliers affected estimates as predicted from the function. The study demonstrated that intuitive judgments in a complex domain are based on the perception of geometric features of the relevant information. By applying these findings, graphic designers can accurately predict how changes in a display will affect viewers' impressions.},
doi = {10.1037/1076-898x.3.1.3},
keywords = {scatterplots},
publisher = {American Psychological Association ({APA})},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1037%2F1076-898x.3.1.3},
}
@Article{Pollack1960,
author = {Irwin Pollack},
title = {Identification of visual correlational scatterplots.},
journal = {Journal of Experimental Psychology},
year = {1960},
volume = {59},
number = {6},
pages = {351--360},
abstract = {Visual correlation scattergrams were obtained by mixing a common noise source with independent noise sources and displaying the mixtures across the coordinates of an oscilloscope. The task of S was to identify whether the reference correlation or a higher correlation was presented. The results were interpreted in terms of the task of S as a tester of alternative statistical hypotheses under conditions of varying reliability of the display information.},
doi = {10.1037/h0042245},
keywords = {scatterplots},
publisher = {American Psychological Association ({APA})},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1037%2Fh0042245},
}
@Article{Strahan1978,
author = {R. F. Strahan and C. J. Hansen},
title = {Underestimating Correlation from Scatterplots},
journal = {Applied Psychological Measurement},
year = {1978},
volume = {2},
number = {4},
pages = {543--550},
month = {oct},
abstract = {Eighty subjects estimated the correlation coefficient, r, for each of 13 computer-printed scatter plots. Making judgments were 46 students in a graduate-level statistics course and 34 faculty and graduate students in a department of psychology. The actual correlation values ranged from .010 to .995, with 200 observations in each scatterplot and with the order of scatterplot presentation ran domized. As predicted, subjects underestimated the degree of actual correlation. Also as predicted, but with substantial moderation by a method-of-presentation factor, this underestimation was most pronounced in the middle of the correlational range—between the 0 and 1 extremes. Though perception of correlation was shown not to be veridical (i.e., in terms of r), little support was given one alternative view — its being in terms of r^2.},
doi = {10.1177/014662167800200409},
keywords = {scatterplots},
publisher = {{SAGE} Publications},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1177%2F014662167800200409},
}
@Article{Li2008,
author = {Jing Li and Jean-Bernard Martens and Jarke J van Wijk},
title = {Judging correlation from scatterplots and parallel coordinate plots},
journal = {Information Visualization},
year = {2008},
volume = {9},
number = {1},
pages = {13--30},
month = {may},
abstract = {Scatterplots and parallel coordinate plots (PCPs) can both be used to assess correlation visually. In this paper, we compare these two visualization methods in a controlled user experiment. More specifically, 25 participants were asked to report observed correlation as a function of the sample correlation under varying conditions of visualization method, sample size and observation time. A statistical model is proposed to describe the correlation judgment process. The accuracy and the bias in the judgments in the different conditions are established by interpreting the parameters in this model. A discriminability index is proposed to characterize the performance accuracy in each experi- mental condition. Moreover, a statistical test is applied to derive whether or not the human sensation scale differs from a theoretically optimal (i.e., unbiased) judgment scale. Based on these analyses, we conclude that users can reliably distinguish twice as many different correlation levels when using scatterplots as when using PCPs. We also find that there is a bias towards reporting nega- tive correlations when using PCPs. Therefore, we conclude that scatterplots are more effective than parallel plots in supporting visual correlation analysis},
doi = {10.1057/ivs.2008.13},
keywords = {scatterplots, parallel-coordinates},
publisher = {{SAGE} Publications},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1057%2Fivs.2008.13},
}
@Article{Tremmel1995,
author = {Lothar Tremmel},
title = {The Visual Separability of Plotting Symbols in Scatterplots},
journal = {Journal of Computational and Graphical Statistics},
year = {1995},
volume = {4},
number = {2},
pages = {101--112},
month = {jun},
abstract = {
Which symbols should be used to represent different groups of data in the same scatterplot? Hypotheses are derived to predict which symbol pairs should lead to good separability, based on the contrast of the symbols' visual properties or "features." In two experiments, experimental scatterplots were shown to subjects on a computer screen; the dependent variable was the decision time to judge which of the two presented symbols was the more frequent one. Analyses of the within-subject effects yielded the following results: (1) Important feature contrasts are brightness, number of line endings, and cur- vature. (2) Symbols that differ simultaneously in two feature dimensions may be more separable than symbols that differ only in either one. (3) The contrasts between circular symbols and radial line symbols like the plus sign or the asterisk are excellent. Practical applications of these findings are discussed, as well as their contribution to the theory of visual perception.},
doi = {10.1080/10618600.1995.10474669},
keywords = {scatterplots},
publisher = {Informa {UK} Limited},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1080%2F10618600.1995.10474669},
}
@Article{Gillan2000,
author = {Douglas J. Gillan and Anna Burd Callahan},
title = {A Componential Model of Human Interaction with Graphs: {VI}. Cognitive Engineering of Pie Graphs},
journal = {Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society},
year = {2000},
volume = {42},
number = {4},
pages = {566--591},
month = {dec},
abstract = {This paper proposes and tests the following three-component model of reading a pie graph to estimate segment size: (a) selecting a mentally represented anchor segment (25%, 50%, or 75%), (b) mentally aligning representations of the anchor and target segments, and (c) mentally adjusting the size of the anchor to match the target. Experiment 1 showed that the size difference between the target and closest anchor and the angular displacement of the target from vertical predicted response times (RTs) and absolute error. Experiment 2 demonstrated that an aligned pie graph, designed to reduce the "align" portion of the process, produced faster RTs and lower error than did a regular pie graph. Experiment 3 showed that a pie graph labeled at the anchor values produced the same response times and absolute error as a regular pie graph but that a pie labeled off the anchor points produced a very different pattern of results. The discussion relates the results to the componential model and describes applications in increasing pie graph usability and developing design guidelines. Actual or potential applications of this research include guidelines for graph design and more usable pie graphs.
},
doi = {10.1518/001872000779698024},
keywords = {scatterplots, pie-charts},
publisher = {{SAGE} Publications},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1518%2F001872000779698024},
}
@InProceedings{Bateman2010,
author = {Scott Bateman and Regan Mandryk and Carl Gutwin and Aaron Genest and David McDine and Christopher Brooks},
title = {Useful Junk? The Effects of Visual Embellishment on Comprehension and Memorability of Charts},
booktitle = {ACM Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (CHI 2010)},
year = {2010},
pages = {2573-2582},
address = {Atlanta, GA, USA},
note = {Best paper award},
abstract = {Guidelines for designing information charts often state that the presentation should reduce 'chart junk' - visual embellishments that are not essential to understanding the data. In contrast, some popular chart designers wrap the presented data in detailed and elaborate imagery, raising the questions of whether this imagery is really as detrimental to understanding as has been proposed, and whether the visual embellishment may have other benefits. To investigate these issues, we conducted an experiment that compared embellished charts with plain ones, and measured both interpretation accuracy and long-term recall. We found that people's accuracy in describing the embellished charts was no worse than for plain charts, and that their recall after a two-to-three-week gap was significantly better. Although we are cautious about recommending that all charts be produced in this style, our results question some of the premises of the minimalist approach to chart design.
},
keywords = {chartjunk},
}
@Article{Borgo2012,
author = {R. Borgo and A. Abdul-Rahman and F. Mohamed and P. W. Grant and I. Reppa and L. Floridi and Min Chen},
title = {An Empirical Study on Using Visual Embellishments in Visualization},
journal = {{IEEE} Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics},
year = {2012},
volume = {18},
number = {12},
pages = {2759--2768},
month = {dec},
abstract = {In written and spoken communications, figures of speech (e.g., metaphors and synecdoche) are often used as an aid to help convey abstract or less tangible concepts. However, the benefits of using rhetorical illustrations or embellishments in visualization have so far been inconclusive. In this work, we report an empirical study to evaluate hypotheses that visual embellishments may aid memorization, visual search and concept comprehension. One major departure from related experiments in the literature is that we make use of a dual- task methodology in our experiment. This design offers an abstraction of typical situations where viewers do not have their full attention focused on visualization (e.g., in meetings and lectures). The secondary task introduces “divided attention”, and makes the effects of visual embellishments more observable. In addition, it also serves as additional masking in memory-based trials. The results of this study show that visual embellishments can help participants better remember the information depicted in visualization. On the other hand, visual embellishments can have a negative impact on the speed of visual search. The results show a complex pattern as to the benefits of visual embellishments in helping participants grasp key concepts from visualization.},
doi = {10.1109/tvcg.2012.197},
keywords = {chartjunk},
publisher = {Institute of Electrical and Electronics Engineers ({IEEE})},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1109%2Ftvcg.2012.197},
}
@Article{Borkin2015,
author = {M. Borkin and Z. Bylinski and N, Kim and C. Bainbridge and C. Yeh and D. Borkin and Pfister H. and A. Oliva},
title = {Beyond Memorability: Visualization Recognition and Recall},
journal = {IEEE Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics},
year = {2015},
volume = {PP},
pages = {1--1},
issn = {1077-2626},
abstract = {In this paper we move beyond memorability and investigate how visualizations are recognized and recalled. For this study we labeled a dataset of 393 visualizations and analyzed the eye movements of 33 participants as well as thousands of participantgenerated text descriptions of the visualizations. This allowed us to determine what components of a visualization attract peopletextquoterights attention, and what information is encoded into memory. Our findings quantitatively support many conventional qualitative design guidelines, including that (1) titles and supporting text should convey the message of a visualization, (2) if used appropriately, pictograms do not interfere with understanding and can improve recognition, and (3) redundancy helps effectively communicate the message. Importantly, we show that visualizations memorable textquotedblleftat-a-glancetextquotedblright are also capable of effectively conveying the message of the visualization. Thus, a memorable visualization is often also an effective one.},
doi = {10.1109/TVCG.2015.2467732},
keywords = {chartjunk},
lines = {1--13},
url = {http://ieeexplore.ieee.org.ezp-prod1.hul.harvard.edu/xpl/articleDetails.jsp?arnumber=7192646\&newsearch=true\&queryText=Beyond\%20Memorability:\%20Visualization\%20Recognition\%20and\%20Recall},
}
@Article{Borkin2013,
author = {Michelle A. Borkin and Azalea A. Vo and Zoya Bylinskii and Phillip Isola and Shashank Sunkavalli and Aude Oliva and Hanspeter Pfister},
title = {What Makes a Visualization Memorable?},
journal = {{IEEE} Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics},
year = {2013},
volume = {19},
number = {12},
pages = {2306--2315},
month = {dec},
abstract = {An ongoing debate in the Visualization community concerns the role that visualization types play in data understanding. In human cognition, understanding and memorability are intertwined. As a first step towards being able to ask questions about impact and effectiveness, here we ask: “What makes a visualization memorable?” We ran the largest scale visualization study to date using 2,070 single-panel visualizations, categorized with visualization type (e.g., bar chart, line graph, etc.), collected from news media sites, government reports, scientific journals, and infographic sources. Each visualization was annotated with additional attributes, including ratings for data-ink ratios and visual densities. Using Amazon’s Mechanical Turk, we collected memorability scores for hundreds of these visualizations, and discovered that observers are consistent in which visualizations they find memorable and forgettable. We find intuitive results (e.g., attributes like color and the inclusion of a human recognizable object enhance memorability) and less intuitive results (e.g., common graphs are less memorable than unique visualization types). Altogether our findings suggest that quantifying memorability is a general metric of the utility of information, an essential step towards determining how to design effective visualizations.},
doi = {10.1109/tvcg.2013.234},
keywords = {chartjunk},
publisher = {Institute of Electrical and Electronics Engineers ({IEEE})},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1109%2Ftvcg.2013.234},
}
@TechReport{Bederson1998,
author = {Benjamin B. Bederson and Angela Boltman},
title = {Does Animation Help Users Build Mental Maps of Spatial Information},
year = {1998},
number = {http://www.cs.umd.edu/hcil},
abstract = {We examine how animating a viewpoint change in a spatial information system affects a user’s ability to build a mental map of the information in the space. We found that animation improves users' ability to reconstruct the information space, with no penalty on task performance time. We believe that this study provides strong evidence for adding animated transitions in many applications with fixed spatial data where the user navigates around the data space.},
keywords = {animation},
}
@Article{Archambault2011,
author = {D Archambault and H Purchase and B Pinaud},
title = {Animation, Small Multiples, and the Effect of Mental Map Preservation in Dynamic Graphs},
journal = {{IEEE} Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics},
year = {2011},
volume = {17},
number = {4},
pages = {539--552},
month = {apr},
abstract = {In this paper, we present the results of a human-computer interaction experiment that compared the performance of the animation of dynamic graphs to the presentation of small multiples and the effect that mental map preservation had on the two conditions. Questions used in the experiment were selected to test both local and global properties of graph evolution over time. The data sets used in this experiment were derived from standard benchmark data sets of the information visualization community. We found that small multiples gave significantly faster performance than animation overall and for each of our five graph comprehension tasks. In addition, small multiples had significantly more errors than animation for the tasks of determining sets of nodes or edges added to the graph during the same timeslice, although a positive time-error correlation coefficient suggests that, in this case, faster responses did not lead to more errors. This result suggests that, for these two tasks, animation is preferable if accuracy is more important than speed. Preserving the mental map under either the animation or the small multiples condition had little influence in terms of error rate and response time.},
doi = {10.1109/tvcg.2010.78},
keywords = {animation},
publisher = {Institute of Electrical and Electronics Engineers ({IEEE})},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1109%2Ftvcg.2010.78},
}
@Article{Chevalier2014,
author = {Fanny Chevalier and Pierre Dragicevic and Steven Franconeri},
title = {The Not-so-Staggering Effect of Staggered Animated Transitions on Visual Tracking},
journal = {{IEEE} Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics},
year = {2014},
volume = {20},
number = {12},
pages = {2241--2250},
month = {dec},
abstract = {Interactive visual applications often rely on animation to transition from one display state to another. There are multiple animation techniques to choose from, and it is not always clear which should produce the best visual correspondences between display elements. One major factor is whether the animation relies on staggering-an incremental delay in start times across the moving elements. It has been suggested that staggering may reduce occlusion, while also reducing display complexity and producing less overwhelming animations, though no empirical evidence has demonstrated these advantages. Work in perceptual psychology does show that reducing occlusion, and reducing inter-object proximity (crowding) more generally, improves performance in multiple object tracking. We ran simulations confirming that staggering can in some cases reduce crowding in animated transitions involving dot clouds (as found in, e.g., animated 2D scatterplots). We empirically evaluated the effect of two staggering techniques on tracking tasks, focusing on cases that should most favour staggering. We found that introducing staggering has a negligible, or even negative, impact on multiple object tracking performance. The potential benefits of staggering may be outweighed by strong costs: a loss of common-motion grouping information about which objects travel in similar paths, and less predictability about when any specific object would begin to move. Staggering may be beneficial in some conditions, but they have yet to be demonstrated. The present results are a significant step toward a better understanding of animation pacing, and provide direction for further research.},
doi = {10.1109/tvcg.2014.2346424},
keywords = {animation},
publisher = {Institute of Electrical and Electronics Engineers ({IEEE})},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1109%2Ftvcg.2014.2346424},
}
@Article{Heer2007,
author = {Jeffrey Heer and George Robertson},
title = {Animated Transitions in Statistical Data Graphics},
journal = {{IEEE} Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics},
year = {2007},
volume = {13},
number = {6},
pages = {1240--1247},
month = {nov},
abstract = {In this paper we investigate the effectiveness of animated transitions between common statistical data graphics such as bar charts, pie charts, and scatter plots. We extend theoretical models of data graphics to include such transitions, introducing a taxonomy of transition types. We then propose design principles for creating effective transitions and illustrate the application of these principles in DynaVis, a visualization system featuring animated data graphics. Two controlled experiments were conducted to assess the efficacy of various transition types, finding that animated transitions can significantly improve graphical perception.},
doi = {10.1109/tvcg.2007.70539},
keywords = {animation},
publisher = {Institute of Electrical and Electronics Engineers ({IEEE})},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1109%2Ftvcg.2007.70539},
}
@Article{Talbot2014,
author = {Justin Talbot and Vidya Setlur and Anushka Anand},
title = {Four Experiments on the Perception of Bar Charts},
journal = {{IEEE} Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics},
year = {2014},
volume = {20},
number = {12},
pages = {2152--2160},
month = {dec},
abstract = {Bar charts are one of the most common visualization types. In a classic graphical perception paper, Cleveland & McGill studied how different bar chart designs impact the accuracy with which viewers can complete simple perceptual tasks. They found that people perform substantially worse on stacked bar charts than on aligned bar charts, and that comparisons between adjacent bars are more accurate than between widely separated bars. However, the study did not explore why these differences occur. In this paper, we describe a series of follow-up experiments to further explore and explain their results. While our results generally confirm Cleveland & McGill's ranking of various bar chart configurations, we provide additional insight into the bar chart reading task and the sources of participants' errors. We use our results to propose new hypotheses on the perception of bar charts.},
doi = {10.1109/tvcg.2014.2346320},
keywords = {barcharts},
publisher = {Institute of Electrical and Electronics Engineers ({IEEE})},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1109%2Ftvcg.2014.2346320},
}
@Article{Barfield1989,
author = {Woodrow Barfield and Robert Robless},
title = {The effects of two- or three-dimensional graphics on the problem-solving performance of experienced and novice decision makers},
journal = {Behaviour & Information Technology},
year = {1989},
volume = {8},
number = {5},
pages = {369--385},
month = {oct},
abstract = {An experiment was performed to investigate the relationship between iwo-dimensional (2-D) or three-dimensional (3-D) graphs displayed on paper or computer and the problem-solving performance of experienced and novice managers. The effects ofthese variables on solution times, confidence in answers and effectivenessof solutions for a production management case were examined. It was predicted that experienced managers would engage in forward chaining as a problem-solving strategy, while novices would use backward chaining as a problem-solvingtechnique (Larkin et al. 1980).Results indicated that solution times were faster for computer than for paper presentations of data, but no significant relationship between response times and dimensionality of graphs was found. Novice subjects produced more accurate answers using 2-D paper presentations of graphs, while experienced managers produced more accurate answers when provided with 3-D graphs on computer. Further, experienced and novice managers were more confident of their answers when provided 2-D graphs as decision aids than with any other mode of presentation. Verbal protocols and retrospective reports indicated that in solving the cases experienced managers engaged in forward chaining, backward chaining and means-ends analysis as problem-solving techniques more often than novices.},
doi = {10.1080/01449298908914567},
keywords = {3D},
publisher = {Informa {UK} Limited},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1080%2F01449298908914567},
}
@Article{Carswell1991,
author = {C. Melody Carswell and Sylvia Frankenberger and Donald Bernhard},
title = {Graphing in depth: perspectives on the use of three-dimensional graphs to represent lower-dimensional data},
journal = {Behaviour & Information Technology},
year = {1991},
volume = {10},
number = {6},